Home

History of Modern Personality Theory and Research

History of Modern Personality Theory and Research

 

 

History of Modern Personality Theory and Research

Chapter 1
History of Modern Personality Theory and Research
David G. Winter
University of Michigan
Nicole B. Barenbaum
University of the South


Why would you wane to read this chapter? To be sure, any science has a history; but the history of a science, while a legitimate field of scholarly in­quiry, is usually quite separate from the science itself. Few chemists are concerned with Johann Bechers now-discredited phlogiston theory or Dmitri Mendeleyevs life and how he came to construct the periodic table of the elements. If psychological science is concerned with what is true now, why bother with what people used to think was true? Why, then, a history of per­sonality psychology?
USES OF A HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE
We suggest three important reasons for personal­ity psychologists to know something of their history. First, origins are intrinsically interest­ing, even compelling. As Freud (1908/1959b) once suggested, the question "Where did I come


from?" may be the child's "first grand problem of life" (p. 212). Origins are often a critical part of adult identity (witness peoples interest in gene­alogy)- In fact) the history of psychology is of in­creasing interest to psychologists, as manifest in the new APA journal, History of Psychology. In this chapter, then, we offer an outline of the "family history" of personality psychology. •
Second, the study of history can help us avoid making the same journeys over the same terrain, repeating the mistakes of the past. If we know what has gone before, we may discover that some questions have been asked before, perhaps even answered! As the philosopher Santayana (1906) cautioned, "Progress, far from consisting in change, depends on rctcntiveness. . . . When experience is not retained .. . infancy is perpet­ual. Those who cannot remember the past are condemned to repeat it" (p. 284).
Further, any discipline is the way it is because its early workers formulated certain questions and framed certain key concepts. By under-


 


standing how our field evolved and was con­structed—by particular psychologists, in par­ticular social contexts—we gain a broader per­spective on contemporary questions and issues. For example, Gordon Allport's decision to ratify "trait" as the key concept for the newly emerging field of personality (Allport, 1927) had impor­tant consequences in framing such later issues as the ways in which personality is measured, sta­bility versus change, the roles of "personality" and "situation" in the explanation of behavior, and the relationship between trait and other per­sonality constructs such as motive (Winter, John, Stewart, Klohnen, &c Duncan, 1998).
Finally, no science is divorced from its sur­rounding social conditions and values-climate. For example, any attempt to study the psychol­ogy of personality presumes the belief that "per­sonality" is important and worth explaining. This belief, in turn, rests on a philosophical indi­vidualism and a view of the individual as cause or agent. Because Americans are particularly likely to hold such individualistic beliefs and re­luctant to acknowledge the importance of collec­tivities (Bellah, Madsen, Sullivan, Swidler, & Tipton, 1985), it is not surprising that "personal­ity" has been a major focus of psychological in­quiry as well as popular culture in the United States (see Susman, 1979). One observer sug­gested that "no country in the world is so driven by personality as this one" and that Americans have a "hunger to identify with larger-than-life personalities" ("Only spectacular crimes," 1994). Our historical account, therefore, will emphasize developments in the United States. It should be taken as building upon earlier historical handbook chapters by Burnham (1968), Pervin (1990), McAdams (1997) and Runyan (1997)— and, indeed, on those entire earlier handbooks.
NINETEENTH-CENTURY INTELLECTUAL ANTECEDENTS OF PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY
The modern psychological study of personality, which began to flourish during the early decades of the 20th century, has roots in three 19th-cen­tury intellectual themes: a deep belief in indi­vidualism, a pervasive concern with irrationality and the unconscious, and a strong emphasis on measurement. Although these three themes are by no means fully consistent with each other and not every personality psychologist emphasized


all three, they did shape (and limit) the emerging field of personality psychology in important ways that can still be recognized.
Individualism and the "Skin-Bounded" Individual
Personality psychology evolved and flourished in the Western philosophical-political climate of in­dividualism—the belief that individuals are im­portant and unique. Such a discipline would be unlikely to develop in a homogeneous society in which everyone lived through the same small set of life cycles. Thus the cultural historian Burck-hardt (1860/1954) concluded that in the Middle Ages, people were conscious of themselves "only as a member of a race, people, party, family, or corporation—only through some general cate­gory" (p. 101). After the Renaissance, however, people recognized themselves as individuals.
From the individualist perspective, "persons," the very subject matter of personality psychol­ogy, are construed as single bothes, bounded by skin. Anything else (objects and environments, intimate partners, other people, family, solidary groups, communities, institutions, cultures) is construed as "outside," wholly external to the autonomous skin-bounded individual person. From this perspective, they only exist and are understood as internal representations, objects of contractual relations, or encroachments on the individual.1
The individualistic perspective, however, leads to a paradox: Often we do not seem so very dif­ferent from each other. For example, the many stuthes of bogus personality feedback (Dickson & Kelly, 1985) suggest that people readily accept uniform, standardized feedback as an accurate description of their own "individual" selves. And Singer (1995), after studying thousands of col­lege students' self-stories, was "overwhelmed by the narrative similarities they bring to the im­portant events in their lives" (p. 452, emphasis added). Further, an excessively individualistic perspective can blind psychologists to the im­portance of collective aspects of personality such as groups, social identities, and cultural symbols.
The Unconscious
In revolt against the 18th-century "age of rea­son," the 19th-century Romantic movement ex­alted the role of unconscious and irrational emo­tions, spontaneity, and impulsivity in literature,



the arts, and philosophy. In personality psychol­ogy, this influence was most obvious in psycho­analysis, particularly Freud's concepts of id and primary process. The notion of an unconscious still survives today, albeit in a muted and less grandiose form, in the claim that many impor­tant processes are implicit or automatic—that is, they operate outside of conscious awareness (Kihlstrom, 1990, and Chapter 17, this volume; see also Bargh, 1982; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). Still, an exclu­sive focus on unconscious or implicit forces may lead to a neglect or underestimation of the im­portance of rational planning and directed be­havior (see Cantor & Zirkel, 1990).
Emphasis on Measurement
In devoting an extraordinary amount of atten­tion to issues of measurement and psyehomct-rics, personality psychology tried to follow in the footsteps of the prestigious "exact sciences" that had developed so rapidly in the late 19th cen­tury. The modern emphasis on precision and measurement was prefigured in the writings of British scientist and statistician Sir Francis Gal-ton (1884): "The character which shapes our conduct is a definite and durable 'something,' and therefore ... it is reasonable to attempt to measure it" (p. 179).2 In the early 20th century, American experimental psychologist £. L. Thorndike (1914) insisted that "if a thing exists, it exists in some amount; and if it exists in some amount, it can be measured" (p. 141; emphasis in original).
The prestige of Binct and the early intelligence "tests" also increased the concern with measure­ment in personality psychology. A rapidly devel­oping assessment technology led to a number of paper and pencil "tests" of personality, modeled after the early intelligence tests and consisting of questionnaire "items" intended to measure the "traits" of personality. Later on, personality psy­chologists expanded their measurement technol­ogy to include ratings and behavior observations.
The emphasis on measurement also fit with the imperative, strongly felt by personality psy­chology in its beginning decades, to be useful, to furnish assistance to a corporate culture and a government suddenly confronted by dramatic changes and the need to "manage" and control an American population that had suddenly become larger, more diverse, and "difficult" (Parker, 1991). This point is nicely illustrated by


the example of Woodworths Personal Data Sheet, which was probably the first personality test based on the IQ test model of adding "scores" on discrete individual "items" to get a total. By the time the United States finally en­tered the war in April 1917, the experience of other armies suggested that many solthers were vulnerable to "shell shock" or "war neurosis." (Nowadays this would probably be called post-traumatic stress disorder.) The American Psycho­logical Association quickly set up a committee charged with developing a diagnostic test of "susceptibility to shock," which was conceived as one aspect of a more general emotional instabil­ity (Camfield, 1969, pp. 126, 131-132). Acting without the benefit of much prior knowledge or technique, Robert Woodworth collected a long list of symptoms from the case histories of "neu­rotic subjects" and turned them into a series of 116 simple questions that could be answered Yes or No (for example, "Do you usually feel well and strong?" or "Has your family always treated you right?"). Overall scores, calculated by sum­ming scores of the individual items, differenti­ated "normal" solthers from diagnosed neurotics or returned "shell shock" cases (Woodwoith, 1919, 1932, p. 374). The resulting Personal Data Sheet was the first objective, self-report "inventory" purporting to measure a personality characteristic, in this case what was later labeled Neuroticism.
A focus on measurement was obviously bene­ficial to any science that aspired to prestige by developing along the traditional positivistic path blazed by 19th-century physics. Conversely, it was a handicap insofar as it constrained the scope of investigation and explanation to that which was easily measured, thereby neglecting more subtle and complex personality charac­teristics and processes. For example, self-report questionnaires constructed on the iQ-test "item" model are sensitive only to conscious (rather than implicit) sentiments. They are usu­ally based on the atomistic assumption that a complex whole can be broken down into a series of small component parts, each of which is equal to every other and all of which combine in addi­tive (rather than interactive or nonlinear) ways. In this respect, the "measurement imperative" of personality psychology sometimes resembled the famous joke about the drunken man who had lost his keys on the dark side of the street but looked for them on the (other) lighted side, "be­cause that's where the light is."


 


THE TWO TASKS OF PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY
From its earliest days, American personality psy­chology involved two related but contrasting en­deavors: (1) the study of individual difference^ or the dimensions along which people differ from each other, and (2) the study of individual persons as unique, integrated wholes (see, e.g., Lamiell, 1997; McAdams, 1997; Murphy, 1932; Sanford, 1963). These two endeavors have been variously labeled as "analytic" versus "structural," or "quantitative" versus "qualitative," respectively. According to one early commentator (Young, 1928):
A review of the whole gamut of personality stuthes . . . reveals two essentially distinct approaches to the data. One of these, which has been developed by recent psychology, concerns itself with a ... cross-sectional treatment of personality in terms of traits, attitudes and habits. The other, which has arisen from a number of sources, especially psy­chiatry, treats personality from a functional, historical-genetic standpoint, (p. 431)
Much of the difference between these two ap­proaches is also captured by the "nomothetic-idiographic" dichotomy—terms first used by Windelband (1894/1904), later adopted by Allport (1937) ,3 and still a lively topic at the end of the century (West, 1983; see also Lamiell, 1998).
The Psychometric "Analysis" Approach
Discussing the analytic or quantitative approach, Murphy (1932) characterized its view of person­ality as the "sum of all of an individuals traits" (p. 386). In his view, psychologists who meas­ured and stuthed the intercorreladons of separate personality traits conceived of personality "as the answer to a complicated arithmetical problem" (p. 386). The practical goal of personality re­search was to predict, modify, and control be­havior, with individual differences conceived as coefficients to be supplied to linear, additive pre­diction equations.4

 

Influence of Intelligence Testing
We have called this the "psychometric" approach because, according to an early reviewer, it "owes its prominence to the work of Gallon, Pearson, Cattell, Thorndike and Terman with their inves-


tigations of individual differences, particularly in the field of intelligence" (Young, 1928, p. 431). During the first two decades of the 20ih century, psychologists had developed "mental tests" for selection, diagnosis, and placement, as psycholo­gists tried to demonstrate their usefulness in solving urgent practical problems associated with immigration, labor unions, and schools, as well as the 1917-1918 national war mobiliza­tion (Danziger, 1990; Parker, 1991; Vernon, 1933; see Schaffcr, 1991, pp. 133-139, on the effects of World War I in particular).
In the post-World War I period, however, many critics questioned the predictive utility of intelligence tests in various applied settings (Parker, 1991) and suggested that measures of personality or character traits were needed to im­prove the prediction of military, managerial, in­dustrial, and educational performance (see, e.g., Fernald, 1920; PorTcnbcrger, 1922; Pressey, 1921). Still, these newer "character tests" were based on the mental test model in preference to other, less "efficient" methods (Parker, 1991). The content of trait measures also reflected prac­tical demands: "Ascendance-Submission" and "Extraversion-Imroversion," considered relevant to selecting business managers and military offi­cers, received the greatest attention (Danziger, 1990; Parker, 1991).
"Mental Hygiene" and Personality
Yet another important influence on the psy­chometric approach to personality during the 1920-1930 period was the so-called mental hy­giene movement. This well-funded alliance of psychiatrists, educators, and social workers viewed individual maladjustment as the root cause of a wide variety of social and personal problems (S. Cohen, 1983; see also Parker, 1991). With its "personality" focus, the move­ment enlisted psychologists to supply a "scien­tific" basis for the therapeutic efforts of mental hygiene workers. As Danziger (1990) noted, "in practice this generally came down to the con­struction and application of scales diat would subject 'personality' to the rigors of measure­ment and so convert it from merely an object of social intervention to an object of science" (p. 164).
"Science" or "Servant"?
The continuing concern of personality psy­chologists to demonstrate the practical useful-



ness of their work on measuring individual dif­ferences suggests the need for viewing history from a political and moral perspective. In fact, most of the "practical social problems" personal­ity psychologists tried to solve were problems faced (and framed) by elite groups: selecting managers who could maximize profits; select­ing military officers who could win wars; con­trolling an increasingly diverse population in the country's schools, factories, crowded cities, and prisons; and controlling "deviant" behavior, at least to the extent of promoting labels suggest­ing individual pathology instead of social prob­lems. Not surprisingly, the "solutions" sup­plied by personality psychologists tended to leave existing power relations intact or even rein­forced, because the intellectual roots of personal­ity psychology—radical individualism, sup­ported by the mystique of "science"—were quite compatible with the ideological stance of the ruling elites who financed the research. Thus the practically oriented psychometric approach to personality psychology runs the danger of be­coming a technology that deliberately or unwit­tingly functions mainly to serve the interests of the powerful.5
The Psychiatric and Historical "Interpretation" Approach
Murphy (1932) contrasted the psychometric ap­proach to personality with the psychiatric focus on "personality as a whole," observing that psy­chiatric conceptions "added much to the rich­ness of the term 'personality"9 by introducing topics such as dissociation and unconscious mental processes (p. 387). Young (1928) was es­pecially enthusiastic about this historical-inter­pretive approach (though he noted that at the time of his writing, it "has had very little atten­tion from the psychologist," p. 437): "[It] has arisen from the study of literary biography, from historical biography, but especially from psychia­try and sociology. Here we find the great biogra­phers, the psychoanalysts led by Freud, Jung and Adler, the invaluable work of Healy [a neurolo­gist] and latterly the sociological reformulation of W. I. Thomas" (p. 431).
Mainstream psychologists ignored or criti­cized biographical and case study methods and were (at least initially) quite hostile toward psy­choanalysis (Danziger, 1990, 1997, especially p. 125). Given their concern for scientific re­spectability via sophisticated quantitative tech­niques, it is not surprising that they viewed the


case study methods of psychiatrists and psycho­analysts as part of an "old-fashioned and unsci­entific" medical tradition (Hale, 1971, p. 115).
Discussing the case study literature in their re­view of personality, Allport and Vcrnon (1930) noted the contributions of psychiatrists and soci­ologists (rather than psychologists) and echoed the prevailing view that the method was "unsat­isfactory." However, they went on to suggest mat "the concrete individual has eluded study by any other approach" and concluded with the hope that "in the future there will undoubtedly be at­tempts to standardize the case-study in some way which will reduce its dependence upon the uncontrolled artistry of the author" (p. 700).
The whole-person or interpretive approach to personality had been especially prominent in German psychology during the first three dec­ades of the 20th century. Influenced by the phi­losopher Dilthey s view of psychology as a "hu­man science" (Geisteswiuenschafi), it emphasized the ways in which personality characteristics and other psychological processes were organized or patterned within the unique individual. Writing for an American authence about the German qualitative approach to the "undivided" person­ality, Allport summarized this perspective: "More fundamental than differential psychology [i.e., the psychometric focus on dimensions of difference among people], by far, is the problem of the nature, the activity, and the unity of the total personality" (Allport, 1923, p. 614; empha­sis in original). Allport traced the German em­phasis on structured "wholes" in Gestalt psy­chology, the personalistic psychology of William Stern, and Eduard Spranger's method of "intui­tive understanding" (Ventehen) (Allport, 1923, 1924a, 1924b, 1929).
However, Allports early attempts to introduce this approach to personality to American psy­chologists apparently met with little response. Vernon (1933) noted that most American psy-chometrists were either unaware of the German qualitative approach or else found it scientifically unacceptable.
The Uneasy Coalition between "Analysis" and "Interpretation" Approaches
For the past 70 years, the two approaches to per­sonality psychology—the study of individual personality differences and the study of individual persons—have existed in an uneasy coalition or truce. As part of their efforts to broaden (and systematize) personality, Allport (1937) and



Murray (1938) tried to move the field toward studying persons as integrated wholes, but as several later commentators noted, personality psychologists generally avoided stuthes of indi­vidual lives through most of the century (see, e.g., Carlson, 1971; McAdams & West, 1997; Runyan, 1997).
Even Allport and Murray seemed ambivalent about case stuthes. As a journal editor and pro­fessor, Allport advocated studying individual per­sons (sec, e.g., Allport, 19245, 1929, 1937, 1942) and tried (unsuccessfully) to develop a set of systematic rules for writing life histories (Allport, 1967; see also Barenbaum, 1997a, 1997c; Garracy, 1981). Yet he himself actually published only one case study (Allport, 1963), and much of his research involved nomo-thetic traits (Allport, 1928; Vernon & Allport, 1931). Situated within Harvard's scientifically oriented academic department of psychology, Allport opted for a moderate, eclectic approach that included both analytic and interpretive methods (sec Nicholson, 1997a, but also Pan­dora, 1997).
Situated as a maverick in opposition to the "scientistic" Harvard department (Robinson, 1992), Murray still needed to justify his founda­tion support on the basis of the "scientific" status of his work (see also Triplet, 1983, 1992). Although Murray's group developed elaborate procedures for the intensive study of individual persons, their book (Murray, 1938) actually in­cluded only one case.6 Later, in establishing an assessment program for the wartime Office of Strategic Services (OSS), Murray modified his "diagnostic council" method even more in the direction of more quantitative assessments (McLeod, 1992).
From a historical perspective, it is easy to see that the psychometric tradition was already well established in psychology by the time that per­sonality emerged as a separate field, so that this prior "triumph of the aggregate" (Danziger, 1997, p. 68) led to an early dominance of the "individual differences" tradition over the "study of individuals" tradition. For example, when the American Psychological Association took over publication of the Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology (from the psycruatrically oriented American Psychopathological Association) in 1925, the publication pattern changed dramati­cally (Danziger, 1990):
What was given up was an earlier tradition of de­tailed study of individuals.... Whereas in 1924[,]


80 percent of the empirical papers published were based on the study of individual cases, that propor­tion dropped to 25 percent in the following year. Their place was taken by statistical stuthes based on group data, as was required by the prevailing Galtonian paradigm for psychological measure­ment, (p. 165)
The marginal status of stuthes of individual per­sons in personality psychology, then, really con­tinues an early trend. Yet interest in individual cases has continued to survive—at first mainly among students and associates of Allport and Murray (e.g., Polansky, 1941; Rosenzweig, 1943; White, 1952), but in recent years among a much broader and larger array of personality psychologists (Runyan, 1997, p. 42; see the re­view by McAdams & West, 1997; also the col­lections edited by Franz & Stewart, 1994, and McAdams & Ochberg, 1988)7
1921-1946: THE CRITICAL FORMATIVE ERA OF PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY IN THE UNITED STATES
We suggest that while the "official" emergence of personality psychology is often dated by the ap­pearance of the "canonical" texts by Allport (1937), Murray (1938),8 and Stagner (1937), it really began at least 15 years earlier. The 1921-1938 period was a time of intense research activ­ity; the years between 1938 and 1946 were really the consolidation of "modern" personality con­cepts and methods, as well as the widespread in­stitutional recognition of personality as a spe­cialty within psychology departments (Danziger, 1990; 1997; Parker, 1991). We first review the period before the canonical texts, then discuss the latter period in terms of the influence of three major figures who had substantial and en­during impact on the field: Gordon Allport (1897-1967), Henry Murray (1893-1988), and Raymond Cattell (1905-1998).
Origins: The Decade of the 1920s
The first American review of the psychological literature on "personality and character" ap­peared in 1921 (Allport, 1921).9 Most of the various sources cited by Allport involved traits, which suggests that the trait concept had already achieved a theoretical dominance by that time (Danziger, 1990; Parker, 1991). The article



focused primarily on the distinction between "personality" and "character," two concepts that had been used interchangeably by American psy­chologists up to that time. In agreement with the behaviorists, Allport suggested that the latter term, defined as "the personality evaluated ac­cording to prevailing standards of conduct" (p. 443), was not an appropriate topic for psy­chological study. Allport continued to advocate the use of "personality" in preference to "charac­ter" (1927; Allport & Vernon, 1930); this soon became standard practice, not only for reasons of scientific respectability but also on account of a cultural shin from moralism to pragmatism (Nicholson, 1998).
A "Personality"Journal
Before the 1920s, the term "personality" had been used primarily in discussions of abnormal psychology, which was considered to be the province of the medical specialty of psychiatry (Parker, 1991). Another indication that the field of personality was emerging as a separate and autonomous area of psychological research was the 1921 expansion of the Journal of Abnormal Psychology into the Journal of Abnormal Psychol­ogy ana Social Psychology™ with Floyd Allport as an additional editor "in cooperation with" Morion Prince. In a joint editorial, they noted that psychopathologists and social psycholo­gists shared an interest in the "dynamics of hu­man nature" and invited contributions on a number of topics, including "the foundation-study of human traits" and "the personality of the individual" (Prince &: Allport, 1921, p. 2). The lead article of the first issue of the ex­panded journal was the study of personality traits by the Allport brothers (Allport & Allport, 1921).
Seven years later, Prince and Moore (1928) in­troduced a special issue of the Journal of Abnor­mal and Social Psychology devoted to personality with the observation that "the rapidly increasing number of manuscripts dealing with problems of temperament, character and personal traits shows that an extraordinary pressure of investi­gation is being directed toward this subject which until quite recently numbered but a few brief pages in any standard textbook of psychol­ogy." They noted the "necessity of ever widening approaches and more manifold stuthes," and concluded with a prophetic reference to "the complex task that faces the author of a Psychol­ogy of Personality ten years hence" (p. 117).


Institutional Developments
By the end of the 1920s there were several insti­tutional indicators of the widespread interest in personality as a field of psychology. During the period 1923-1928, the American Psychological Association responded to its members' increas­ing interest in personality by scheduling conven­tion sessions on "character" and "personality," sometimes in the practical context of vocational guidance or selection (Parker, 1991). Similar de­velopments occurred at other conferences and meetings. Support for personality research came from a variety of foundations. Other signs of recognition of the new discipline included the founding of the journal Character and Personality (later renamed Journal of Personality) in 1932, and the 1934 addition or personality" as a cate­gory in the Psychological Abstracts (Parker, 1991).
Allport: Defining and Systematizing the Field of Personality
Gordon Allport s early efforts to define and sys­tematize the field of personality included what he later described as "perhaps the first American dissertation written explicitly on the question of component traits of personality" (Allport, 1967, p. 9}.n Trained in social ethics as well as psychol­ogy, Allport attempted in his 1922 dissertation to define "a sound conception of human person­ality" on which to base "effective social service," arguing that "sound theory must underlie appli­cation" (Allport, 1967, p. 7). Rejecting as super­ficial many of the existing trait measures derived from exclusively practical concerns, he adopted a behavioristic definition of traits as "systems of habits" and designed measures of those traits he saw as basic components of personality.
As a postdoctoral student in Germany (1922-1923), Allport encountered psychological ap­proaches to personality that "converted" him from his "semifaith in behaviorism" (1967, p. 12). From that time onward, he advocated struc­tural approaches such as Gestalt psychology, Sprangers intuitive method, and Sterns person-alistic emphasis on the uniqueness and unity of personality (see, e.g., Allport, 1923, 1924a, 1924b, 1929). Stern was especially influential in shaping Allport s views (Allport, 1967): "From Stern in particular I learned that a chasm exists between the common variety of differential psy­chology (which he himself had largely invented ...) and a truly personalistic psychology that fo­cuses upon the organization, not the mere profil­ing, of an individuals traits" (p. 10). Allport was


 


particularly impressed with Sterns "repudiation" of his earlier view of "personality as a sum-total of traits" in favor of an emphasis on "the total personality" (Allport, 1924a, p. 359), and Sterns influence is especially reflected in many of Allports later (1937) views (e.g., the very defini­tion of "trait," the concept of functional auton­omy of motives, and the distinction between idiographic and nomothetic methods; see Baren-baum, 1997b).
Returning from Europe in 1924, Allport con­tinued his efforts to define a systematic psychology of personality. Having accepted an offer to teach social ethics at Harvard, he taught what he later described as "probably the first course on the subject [of personality] offered in an Ameri­can college" (Allport, 1967, p. 9). With the title "Personality and Social Amelioration," the course was first offered in 1924 in the Depart­ment of Social Ethics. The following year it was cross-listed in psychology under a new title, "Personality: Its Psychological and Social As­pects" (Nicholson, I997b, pp. 734-735). Dur­ing his time at Dartmouth (from 1926 to 1930) and later at Harvard (in the psychology depart­ment), Allport refined his definition of traits (1931) and attempted to synthesize analytic and interpretive methods of studying personality. He experimented with case methods in teaching (1929) and developed the Study of Values, an in­strument based on Spranger's six fold conception of value (Vernon & Allport, 1931).
Although personality research had prolifer­ated during the 1920s, Allport and other com­mentators noted a continuing lack of attention to the development of personality theory. Ac­cording to Allport and Vernon (1930), "experi­mental approaches in recent years have far out­grown an adequate philosophical and theoretical Background, especially in America" (p. 702). In a similar vein, Murphy (1932) commented that "those who have occupied themselves with the measurement of personality traits have in general been even less concerned with the theory of per­sonality than most intelligence testers have been with theories of intelligence" (p. 386). Vernon (1933) attributed this lack of attention to American psychologists' preoccupation with ap­plied issues: "The preliminary study of funda­mental theoretical problems seems to have been neglected for the sake of practical results" (p. 166).
Allport's 1937 text, like his early reviews, attempted to define and systematize the field of personality psychology in order to provide


"co-ordinating concepts and theories" (1937, p. ix). His survey of existing definitions of per­sonality and methods of studying it led to a renewed emphasis on trait as the fundamental unit of study for personality. Traits, Allport sug­gested, were neuropsychic systems with dynamic or motivational properties. At the same time Allports focus throughout was on "the manifest individuality of mind" (p. vii), which implied both idiographic and nomothetic methods. Al­though some critics considered this focus on the individual unscientific (Bills, 1938; Skaggs,' 1945), most reviewers immediately recognized Allports "precise, well-integrated, and thought­ful review" of the field of personality and his "coherent defense of the individual as a proper subject for scientific study" (Jenkins, 1938, p. 777). Even those who took issue with Allport's views agreed that the book would be in­fluential (Guilford, 1938; Hollingworth, 1938; Jenkins, 1938).
Allport's influence on the emerging field of personality psychology and his anticipation of issues of enduring significance in personality theory and research are well known (see, e.g., Craik, Hogan, & Wolfe, 1993). We have dis­cussed above his efforts to reconcile analytic and interpretive approaches and his emphasis on the structure and organization of the individual per­sonality. Further, on account of his pioneering contribution to the lexical study of traits (Allport & Odbert, 1936), as well his insistence that "a theory of personality requires more than a descriptive taxonomy" of traits, John and Rob­ins (1993) claim Allport as the "father and critic of the Five-Factor Model" (pp. 225, 215), (We mention below the influence of Allports con­cepts of motivation and the self.) Cohler (1993) has succinctly summarized Allport's importance to the field of personality:
Allport's primary contribution to the study of the person may be less a matter of theoretical notions, methodological prescriptions, or empirical work than of his uncanny ability to comprehend the ma­jor issues in the field. As earl/ as his first papers, Allport was aware of the fundamental problems confronting those who wished to study persons, such as the problem of distinguishing between text and interpretation, the advantages and drawbacks of individual difference formulations in the study of personality structure, the fact that traits as well as environments were ever-changing, and the chal­lenges of accounting for continuity and change in lives over time. (p. 142)



Murray: Expanding Disciplinary Boundaries
Henry Murray came to the study of personality by way of psychoanalysis and abnormal psychol­ogy. Trained originally in medicine and in bio­chemistry, Murray chose a career in "depth psy­chology" after encountering the work of Jung and Freud. Only after accepting a position as assistant director of Morton Prince's Harvard Psychological Clinic in 1926, however, did he realize that the academic psychology of the time had very little in common with psychoanalysis (Murray, 1940; Murray, 1967; Robinson, 1992; Triplet, 1992). At a time when psychologists were struggling to define and delimit the disci­plinary boundaries of their field, Murray's unor­thodox and divergent interests were not accept­able to proponents of a strictly scientific psychology; in fact, they almost cost him his po­sition at Harvard (Triplet, 1983).
Murray's eclectic, multimethod approach re­flected his medical training and the nonpositivist philosophy of science of Whitehead and Hen-derson (Laughlin, 1973; Robinson, 1992), as well as the theories of Freud and Jung. Yet at the same time, Murray's background in biochemis­try permeated his work; for example, in the ex­plicit analogy between his classification of the "variables of personality" and the periodic table of chemistry (1938, pp. 142-143). The diffi­culty of such an eclectic enterprise is perhaps in­dicated by Murray's numerous revisions of his system of personality variables (Murray, 1951, 1968, 1977) and by the tentative titles he used for these works (e.g., "Preparations for the Scaf­fold of a Comprehensive System"; Murray, 1959).
While Explorations in Personality was praised in a representative mainstream review (Elliott, 1939) as "by far the most comprehensive at­tempt to bring Freudian psychology and experi­mental psychology into line with each other" (p. 453), the tenor of the review was decidedly mixed. On the one hand, Elliott noted that as a practicing psychoanalyst, Murray was "unique among those who hold a major position in psy­chology in a leading American university" (p. 453). He was impressed with the new tests and procedures (especially the Thematic Apper­ception Test) and with Murray's analysis of the variables of personality and environment. On the other hand, Elliott suggested that psycho­analysis was unproven and unscientific and criti-


cized Murray for overlooking existing work in experimental and differential psychology as well as for insufficient use of statistics. He considered the book's single case study too speculative.
According to Triplet (1983, 1992), Murray's work had a profound effect on the expansion of the disciplinary boundaries of personality psy­chology. By demonstrating the use of experi­mental techniques to investigate psychoanalytic concepts, by teaching courses on abnormal and dynamic psychology in a prestigious academic psychology department, and by inspiring a large number of graduate students, Murray played an important part in expanding the definition of personality psychology to include psychoanalytic theories that had earlier "had pariah status in academia" (Smith, 1990, p. 537). While he ad­vocated the interpretive study of lives, his most enduring legacy was probably the catalog of "variables ofpersonality" (the title of Chapter 3 of Exploration^ see Danziger, 1997) and the Thematic Apperception Test (Morgan & Mur­ray, 1935).
Murray's diagnostic council method was gen­erally overlooked, however (McLeod, 1992). Al­though variants of the approach were used in later assessment settings such as the Office of Strategic Services (OSS), and the Institute for Personality Assessment and Research (IPAR) at the University of California (MacKinnon, 1975), as well as in organizations such as AT&T (Bray, 1982), the practical demands of assess­ment even in those well-financed settings led as­sessors to base decisions "on statistical averages of ratings, rather than on discussion" (McLeod, 1992, p. 10). Perhaps only when executives of the very highest levels (and salaries) were to be assessed, could the full range of person-focused techniques be financially justified.
f
Cartel] and the Measurement Imperative
The third major figure in the formative years of American personality psychology was Raymond Cattell. Cattell was born and educated in Eng­land (with an undergraduate degree in chemistry and a doctorate in psychology). He was greatly influenced by his assisrantships at the University of London with Charles Spearman,12 who first developed the technique of factor analysis, and later at Columbia University with E. L. Thorndike. Most of his professional career was spent at the University of Illinois, where he in­troduced many conceptual refinements and elaborated methodological developments, espe-



daily concerning the use of correlation, factor analysis, and other mulcivariate techniques, into the field of personality. Taking seriously Thorndike's famous dictum about measurement as well as the early work on intelligence testing, Cattell prefaced his first major book on person­ality with the assertion that "it is on measure­ment that all further scientific advance depends" (1946, p. iv). He went on to argue that "the ideal of a science of personality description [is] to build its traits upon a foundation of objective test measurements, as has been done to a very large extent in the analysis of abilities" (p. 210).
Kinds of Data
In reviewing the extant literature on personality measurement, Cattell first distinguished among different types of data (1946, pp. 10, 210-213; chaps. 9-11): ratings by skilled judges (R data), self-ratings (S data), and measurements on tests or experimental situations (T data). (Later he added life-outcome or L data to the list; see Cat-tell, 1957.) He believed that in the long run, T data were the soundest; but since for the foresee­able future objective tests could not measure everything of importance, he preferred ratings (1946, p. 294).
Kinds of Personality Traits
Like Allport, Cattell adopted trait as the funda­mental conceptual unit of personality; but at the same time he also, in the spirit of Murray, distin­guished motivational or "dynamic traits" (also called ergic traits or ergs) from stylistic or "tem­perament traits," as well as "ability traits." In his view, each kind of trait had its own pattern of correlational relationships among its component variables and the external situation: Thus, dy­namic trait variables "change most in response to change of incentives" and showed complex higher-order correlations, while temperament trait variables "change least in response to any change in the field" (1946, p. 167). CattelTs ma­jor contribution to personality was his analysis of temperament traits via mathematical and sta­tistical techniques.
The Search for Personality Coherence through Factor Analysis
Given the enormous array of data available to the personality psychologist—"too many vari­ables, and too many influences behind these


variables" (1946, p. 272)—Cartell turned to cor­relation and orner multivariatc techniques for clarification. Believing that the essence of a trait was covariation or correlation, he concluded that the "most potent method of attacking the tangle is to work out correlation coefficients between the inconveniently multitudinous vari­ables abounding in the subject and to seek some smaller number of 'behind the scenes' or underlying variables, known as factors" (p. 272).
Cattelis first major investigation of personal­ity (1945) used peer ratings of 208 men on 35 trait-word clusters derived from the exhaustive Allport and Odbert (1936) list of 4,504 traits. After carrying out a factor analysis of these data, Cattell stuthed various rotations and finally set­tled on an oblique rotation that yielded 12 fac­tors, which were also consistent widi cluster stuthes carried out by other investigators. (The rotations process, which could now be done on a personal computer in seconds, took 6 months!) He concluded that these 12 factors represented "the established primary traits" (1946, Chaps. 10-12).
Cattell thus introduced or established many conceptions and techniques that are an enduring part of contemporary personality psychology. His distinction between different kinds of data (especially the difference between R and S data, on the one hand, and T data, on the other) re-emerged in the 1970s dispute about the cross-situational and temporal consistency of person­ality (Block, 1977; Epstein, 1979). His formula­tion and defense of the "lexical hypothesis," that "language covers all aspects [of personality] that are important for other human beings" (1946, pp. 215-216),13 prefigured the adjective-based trait assessments widely used over the next several decades. Finally, his discussion and exam­ples of rotated factor analyses set the stage for an entire approach to personality, though there would be considerable controversy about his particular methods and assumptions. For exam­ple, Eysenck preferred orthogonal rather than oblique rotations, and so he argued that Cattell's 12 oblique factors were really equivalent to his own three orthogonal "superfactors" of Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Psychoticism (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985). Yet in spite of Cattells general influence, few personality psy­chologists adopted his full views about personal­ity measurement as outlined in his "specification equation" for predicting behavior (Cattell, 1957, pp. 302-306).


Psychometric Technology
We have seen that the prestige and apparent suc­cess of intelligence testing at the beginning of the 20th century convinced many personal­ity psychologists that personality could (and should) be measured in a similar way, by scales of "items." While factor analysis was the apothe­osis of this ideal, the same general conviction guided the construction of numerous other scales, inventories, and questionnaires not based on factor analysis: omnibus instruments such as the Bernreuter Personality Inventory, Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, California Personality Inventory (CPI; Gough, 1957; 1987), Adjective Check List, and Personality Re­search Form (PRF; Jackson, 1974), as well as countless scales designed to measure particular personality characteristics.
As statistical methods of scale construction and refinement became increasingly sophisti­cated, the psychometric rule book expanded to include matters such as test-retest reliability, in­ternal and cross-situational consistency (Cron-bach, 1951), convergent-discriminant validity (Campbell & Fiske, 1959), correction for at­tenuation, and the distinction between "trait" and "state." Some psychologists protested that psychometric rules unduly constrained personal­ity theorizing and research because they did not take account of nonlinear, interactive, or functionally substitutable (but not correlated) relationships among components of a con­cept (see Atkinson, 1982; Fleming, 1982; Win­ter et al., 1998). At the end of the century, the increased popularity of chaos theory and as­sociated mathematical concepts (e.g., Vallacher & Nowak, 1997) suggested possible alternatives to the classical psychometric rules.
THE FOURFOLD FLOWERING OF PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY: TRAITS, MOTIVES, COGNITIONS, AND CONTEXT
By 1946, shortly after the end of World War II, the main concepts and issues of personality psy­chology were established. For almost all person­ality psychologists, traits were viewed as a major element of personality. Many would say traits were the only personality element (e.g., A. Buss, 1989); others, however, argued for the funda­mental distinctiveness and importance of mo-


tives as well (see Winter et al., 1998). Rating scales and questionnaires were firmly established as the preferred method of personality measure­ment, especially for traits.
Our account of the next 50 years of personal­ity psychology is framed in terms of the four ele­ments or classes of theory and variables and theories introduced by Winter (1996): traits, motives, cognitions, and social context. The arti­cles in the special issue of Ac Journal of Personal­ity on "levels and domains in personality" (Em-mons & McAdams, 1995) offer a similar scheme for organizing personality variables.
PERSONALITY TRAITS
In personality psychology, the concept of trait has been used to denote consistent intcrcorre-lated patterns of behavior, especially expressive or stylistic behavior (see Winter et al., 1998, pp. 232-233). Our discussion of traits will be organized around four rather disparate topics, because these reflect the main lines of theorizing and research about traits. Perhaps the most fre­quently stuthed topic has been the number, nature, and organization of "basic" traits. Per­sonality psychologists have used three different strategies. In approximate order of popularity, they are: factor analysis and related mathemati­cal techniques; rational or a priori theorizing, often involving the construction of typologies; and the idiographic approach, which essentially ignores the question of "basic" traits. After dis­cussing developments in each of these three strategies, we turn to another frequently stuthed topic, namely the relation between traits and biology.
Factor Analytic Study of Traits                    ,
With the availability of cheap, powerful com­puters, factor analysis became the method of choice for understanding traits. By the end of the century, many personality psychologists had reached a working consensus that the trait do­main could be described, at least at the broadest and most abstract level, by five orthogonally ro­tated factors or dusters of traits (see John, 1990; also Chapter 4, this volume; McCrae & Costa, Chapter 5, this volume; Wiggins, 1996), meas­ured in a variety of alternative ways (see Gold-berg, 1992): Extraversion or Surgency, Agree-ablcness, Conscientiousness, Neurotictsm, and Openness (often called the five-factor model or



"Big Five"). Nevertheless, Eysenck and his fol­lowers (Eyscnck & Eysenck, 1985) have contin­ued to argue that the three factors measured by the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ)— Extraversion, Ncuroticism, and Psychoticism— are sufficient (see the exchange between Costa &t McCrae, 1992a, 1992b; and Eysenck, 1992a, 1992b). Block (1995) challenged many of the underlying assumptions (going back to Cattells earliest stuthes) of factor-analytic techniques in general as well as the five-factor model in
particular.

Alternative Analyses of Traits
Rationally and Empirically Constructed Scales
Still, the five-factor model of traits had not achieved complete hegemony at the end of the century. Several personality psychologists devel­oped trait measures based on alternative meth­ods: Gough (1957) developed the CPI by using contrasting groups to construct scales for posi­tive lay or "folk" concepts such as "achieve­ment," "sociability," or "dominance." In a later revision (Gough, 1987), he used clustering tech­niques to construct three "vectors" that bore some resemblance to Eysenck's three factors. Scales of the PRF were labeled with the names of the Murray needs, although they appeared to function essentially as traits.
Typologies
From time to time, personality psychologists have proposed certain syndromes or types, or co­herently organized bundles of personality char­acteristics that define an interesting pattern. The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (Myers, 1962; see also Thorne & Gough, 1991), which in the last decades of the century became one of the most widely used personality tests, was developed on the basis of Jungs (1923/1971) typological com­bination of traits (Extraversion-Introversion) and predominant "functions" (thinking, feeling, sensing, and intuiting).
Jungs typology and Cough's vectors are both comprehensive, that is, presumed to cover all people. Other personality psychologists have used types and typologies in a more limited way, to formalize observations about interest­ing cases or summarize complex data patterns. For example, Freud wrote about the anal type (1908/1959a) and various character types


(1916/1957). Murray (1955/1981) described an "Icarus" type. In recent years, psychologists have used Q-sort methods and inverse (P) factor analysis to define types: for example, Blocks (1971) account of how personality develops over time, Winks (1992) description of different forms of narcissism, and York & Johns (1992) use of Rank's theory to interpret patterns of adult women's lives.
The Idiographic Approach
In spite of the popularity and prestige of factor analysis, the idiographic approach, which rejects the need to search for underlying "basic" trait factors and instead draws upon the broad lexi­con for whatever trait adjectives fit a particular person, has continued to survive (West, 1983; Winter, 1996, Chap. 11).
Biological Basis of Traits
From the time of Galen's famous theory of hu­mors, people have speculated about links be­tween personality, or mind, and biology, or body. For example, Freud anchored his theory in biology, and Murray (1938) defined needs as "physico-chemical" forces in the brain (p. 124). In the mid-20th-century, Eysenck began a sustained effort to link the trait fac­tors of Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Psy­choticism to individual differences in nervous system structures and functioning. Early on (Eysenck, 1957, 1967), he used Pavlovian con­cepts of excitation and inhibition; later (Eysenck & Eyscnck, 1985; Eyscnck, 1990), he turned to arousal and such brain structures as the recicular formation and the limbic sys­tem. Gray (1981, 1987; see also Picketing & Gray, Chapter 10, this volume) proposed an al­ternative factor structure of traits and a corre­spondingly different conception of their bio­logical bases.
Cloninger (1991, 1998) proceeded in the op­posite direction, using individual differences in chemically defined neural pathways to define scales reflecting personality traits. Hcritability stuthes flourished as a way to estimate genetic contributions to trait differences. For all of the progress and excitement, though, definitive and replicable results linking personality and biology still remained somewhat elusive at the end of the century (see D. Buss, 1990; Zuckerman, 1991; and Winter, 1996, Chap. 14).


MOTIVATIONAL CONCEPTS IN PERSONALITY
The personality construct of motive is based on the fundamental postulate that most behavior is oriented toward a£<Wand shows intelligent vari­ation in moving toward the goal (or away from it, with avoidance motives), in response to in­centives, circumstances, opportunities, obstacles, and other current goals. Thus motives contrast widi traits, for as Murray (1938, pp. 56-58) pointed out, a given motive may be associated with an indefinitely large number of quite differ­ent actions; correspondingly, the same action may serve multiple and varied goals (see also Pervin, 1983; 1989; Little, Chapter 20, this vol­ume). Thus the concept of "alpha," which is so essential for consistency-based trait concepts, has litde meaning for motives. In one form or an­other, the distinction between motives and traits appears in the theorizing of many personality psychologists (see Pervin, 1994; Winter et al., 1998).
Motive Concepts in the Psychoanalytic Tradition
Nature and Organization of Motives
Freud placed motivation at the center of person­ality. He argued that all behavior was motivated and grouped human motives into a few broad, general classes (rather than the long lists of spe­cific "instincts" compiled by 19th-century biolo­gists and psychologists): self-preservation; libidi-nal or sexual motives; and aggressive motives or "death instinct" (see Freud 1916-1917/1961-1963, 1920/1955, and 1933/1964). While such a grouping had roots in Western thought as far back as Empedocles (5th century B.C.E), Freud's vigorous insistence on the libidinal (and later, aggressive) motivational roots of human behavior led him to impressive accomplishments in the analysis of surprising and unusual behav­ior, as well as cultural symbols. (Analysis of sym­bolism, often by psychologists who departed from psychoanalytic theory, e.g., Jung, von Franz, Henderson, Jacobi, & Jane, 1964, also made important contributions to cultural stuthes and the humanities.)
Many post-Freudian theorists rephrased Freud's dualistic motivational theory. For exam­ple, Bakan's (1966) concepts of "agency" and "communion" stimulated a good deal of empiri-


cal research (Hclgeson, 1994; Wiggins, 1991; Wiggins & Trobst, Chapter 26, this volume), whereas Winter (1996, Chap. 5) linked Freud's libidinal and aggressive motive groupings to the TAT-measured affiliation and power motives.
Drawing eclectically on psychoanalytic theory and its neo-Freudian variants and basing his conclusions on an intensive study of a group of normal adult males, Murray (1938) constructed an empirically based catalog of 20 needs or mo­tives'4 that has been widely accepted by later per­sonality psychologists, either as a general list measured by questionnaires, such as the PRF, or as the basis for elaborate research programs measuring particular motives (e.g., McClelland, Atkinson, Clark, & Lowell, 1953, on the achievement motive; sec also C Smith, 1992).
Related Personality Concepts Deriving from Psychoanalytic Motivational Theory
Freud's claim that all behavior was motivated naturally entailed the concept of unconscious motives, or motives that were out of awareness. To explain why motives might become uncon­scious, Freud postulated the structures of id, ego, and superego; to answer the question of how, he introduced the notion of defense mecha­nisms that transform libidinal and aggressive motives in varied ways so as to render them "safe," thereby reducing anxiety. Anna Freud (1937/1946) elaborated the nature and opera­tion of the defense mechanisms more fully.
Over the years, psychologists have carried out a good deal of experimental research to evaluate psychoanalytic theory and especially defense mechanisms such as repression (see e.g., Cramer, 1991; Eriksen &: Pierce, 1968; Fisher & Green-berg, 1977; and Sears, 1943). The concept of an "unconscious," however, remained controversial, especially to such behaviorist psychologists as Skinner (1953).I5 However, advances in the study of cognition suggested ways to understand or translate psychoanalytic concepts in terms of sophisticated mechanisms of information proc­essing (Erdclyi, 1974, 1985; see also Kihlstrom, 1990, and Chapter 17, this volume). At the same time, the research program on subliminal psychodynamic activation by Silverman and his colleagues (Weinberger & Silverman, 1990) pro­vided a striking demonstration of unconscious processes as well as specific psychodynamic mo­tives. Some psychologists preferred more theo­retically neutral concepts, such as "implicit"


psychological mechanisms (Grccnwald & Banaji, 1995), to describe these "unconscious" effects.
Intrinsic Motivation
As a reaction against what he considered to be the excessive psychoanalytic (and behaviorist) search for childhood motivational origins of adult behavior, Allport introduced the notion of the "functional autonomy" of motives (1937), by which he meant that the motives actually in­fluencing here-and-now adult behavior are not (or are not any longer) derived from original "primitive" or "primary" drives, such as libido or childhood experiences. More formally, func­tional autonomy presumes an "acquired system of motivation in which the tensions involved are not of the same kind as the antecedent tensions from which the acquired system developed" (Allport, 1961, p. 229).
From the beginning, the concept of func­tional autonomy was criticized, and it is fair to say that personality psychologists have not ac­cepted it. However, Allport's argument certainly anticipated several important motivational de­velopments in the latter decades of the 20th century: (1) the notion that age-graded tasks de­rived from cultural imperatives create "motiva­tion" through such mechanisms as "life tasks," "goals," or "personal projects" (Cantor & Zirkel, 1990); (2) the concept of intrinsic motivation (Deci, 1975), which holds that activity can gen­erate its own motivation; (3) Rogers's (1959) concept of actualization as a motivational force, i.e., that capacities create motivation; and even (4) the notion that personality develops by dis­continuous "stages," or nonlinear reorganiza­tions of what has gone before.
Measuring Motives through Thematic Apperception
Among the many novel assessment procedures introduced in Murray's Explorations in Personal­ity the Thematic Apperception Test or TAT (Morgan & Murray, 1935) is undoubtedly the most famous and widely used. Although psy­chologists developed many ways to interpret and score the TAT (see Gicser & Stein, in press), the empirically derived measures of motivation pio­neered by McClelland deserve special mention (see Winter, 1998), though tiiey have sometimes been completely ignored by some reviewers of personality assessment (e.g., Rorer, 1990, p. 698). Having been trained at Yale University


during the ascendancy of Hulls rigorous behav­iorism, McClelland and his associates used ex­perimental procedures to arouse such motives as hunger and, later, achievement (McClelland et al., 1953), affiliation, and power (see C. Smith, 1992). Motive scoring systems were defined by observing the changes in thematic apperception produced by arousal.
Application to Cognate Fields
Using these thematic apperceptive measures, McClelland expanded the application of person­ality psychology into other social sciences. The achievement motive, for example, emerged as a major personality impetus to entrepreneurship and economic growth (McClelland, 1961; Span* gler, 1992), whereas the power motive is related to charisma and management success (Winter, 1996, Chap. 5). Under certain circumstances, power motivation is associated with sympathetic nervous system arousal and thereby cardiovascu­lar problems, lowered immune system function­ing, and infectious diseases (McClelland, 1989). High power and low affiliation motives are linked to aggression and war (Winter, 1993).
Psychometric Critique
The early popularity of TAT-based motive meas­ures diminished somewhat in reaction to criti­cisms about (1) low internal consistency and temporal reliability, and (2) the consistent lack of correlation between TAT measures and ques­tionnaire measures of the presumed "same" con­struct. For a variety of reasons (see Atkinson, 1982; Winter, 1996, Chap. 5), many methodo­logical canons traditionally applied to measures of traits arc not fully appropriate to motive concepts and thematic apperceptive measures. Weinberger and McClelland (1990) argued that TAT and direct questionnaire measures reflected two fundamentally different motive systems— one unconscious or "implicit," the other con­scious and self-attributed. The meta-analysis by Spangler (1992) supports this conclusion.
COGNITIONS AND PERSONALITY
The "cognitive revolution" of the late 1950s and early 1960s had major effects on personality (see Blake & Ramsey, 1951, for an early review). Kelly (1955) developed a cognitive theory of personality, based on the persons construct sys-


tern and dispensing completely with motivation. Although his direct influence was confined to clinical psychology and consumer psychology (Jankowicz, 1987), his indirect effect on person­ality was great. Several measures involving cogni­tive style and cognitive complexity were inspired by Kellys ideas (Winter, 1996, pp. 198-210),16 and Mischel, a former Kelly student, used cogni­tive concepts to broaden learning theories of personality (Mischel & Shoda, 1995; see also Cantor, 1990).
Attributional Style
The development of causal attribution theory in social psychology led to a parallel concern with attributional style (or explanatory style) as a per­sonality variable (sec Weiner, 1990; Weincr & Graham, Chapter 24, this volume). Although at­tributions are surely related to emotions and mo­tivation, the relationships are complex and prob­ably run in both directions. The explanatory style concept has generated considerable research on depression, performance, health and other significant life outcomes (see Winter, 1996, pp. 269-277).
"Self-Related Personality Variables
Although as far back as the 1930s Allport called attention to the importance of the self17 for per­sonality integration and functioning, the advent of the cognitive revolution brought a prolifera­tion of "self-" related variables in personality psy­chology (see Markus & Cross, 1990; Robins, Norem, Cheek, Chapter 18, this volume): the self-concept (Wylie, 1974-1979), self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1979), self-monitoring (Snyder, 1987), and self-awareness (Carver & Scheier, 1981). Classic early 20th-century concepts of the role of the "generalized other" (George Her­bert Mead) and the "looking-glass self" (Charles Horton Cooley) were blended in with later con­ceptions to create the symbolic interactionism approach (see Gergen & Gordon, 1968). With the advent of postmodernism and the notion of multiple selves (Gergen, 1991) came the related personality concept of a "dialogical self" (Her­mans, Kempen, & van Loon, 1992).
Erikson's (1950/1963) psychosocial elabora­tion of psychoanalysis was most fully developed around the concept of ego identity (Erikson, 1959/1980), which involved congruence be­tween peoples inner sense of self and the exter­nal social definition they receive. Using the iden-


tity concept, Erikson analyzed the lives of his­torical figures, such as Luther (1958) and Gandhi (1969). Meanwhile, Marcia and his col­leagues (Marcia, Waterman, Matteson, Archer, & Orlofsky, 1993) developed methods for meas­uring different aspects of the identity concept. Given its conceptual status as a bridge between the individual and society, identity also proved particularly useful in analyzing social identity, or the role that social variables, such as gender, race, class, and nationality play in the formation of personality (see Gurin, Miller, & Gurin, 1980; Tajfel, 1982). McAdams (1985) has sug­gested a "life story" model of identity, with power and intimacy motivation, as well as ego development, as component variables.
Finally, coincident with the "self psychology" of Kohut (1985) and perhaps Lasch's (1979) analysis of the late-century United States as a "culture of narcissism," came increased research interest in the psychoanalytic concept of narcis­sism Qohn & Robins, 1994; Wink, 1992).
The Social-Political Context of "Cognitive" Research in Personality
Research trends always develop in particular so­cial and political contexts. Since cognitive per­sonality theory focuses on internal processes that are in principle modifiable, rather than on objec­tive conditions of existence, as the major media­tors (if not "causes") of behavior and well-being, it is especially congenial to an individualist (rather than collectivist or contextual) perspec­tive. If we only need to change people's percep­tions, then the existing realities of status, power, and oppression will continue without critique or challenge. Thus Sampson (1981) observed that the emphasis on cognition arose at the height of the Cold War, just after the apogee of McCardiyism—a time in the United 'States when it was dangerous to challenge the social or­der and advocate change. In popular culture, this was the heyday of Norman Vincent Peales best-selling The Power of Positive Thinking (1952), a quasi-religious exhortation to subjective self-transformation instead of political action.
THE SOCIAL CONTEXT OF PERSONALITY
In many different (though apparently unrelated) ways, personality psychologists have long been concerned about context. Early in the 20th cen-


tury, personality came under the sway of behav­iorism and the prestige of experimental research on learning. Later, the culture and personality movement stressed the broader social matrix in which personality is formed. In the late 1960s and 1970s, the situationist critique of personal­ity caused a major crisis in the field and led to a re-examination of fundamental postulates and research mediods. Finally in the 1980s and 1990s, the rise of "cultural psychology" and the influence of feminist and other critical perspec­tives from the humanities, as well as a more global perspective, showed signs of generating new interest in the social and cultural macrocon-texts of personality.
The Influence of Behaviorism
Even as the early personality psychologists were developing questionnaire measures of certain so­cially important traits, the early behaviorists, such as Watson, were trying to reduce personal­ity to conditioning and instrumental learning processes. Watson and Rayners (1920) famous "Little Albert" demonstration had an enduring influence, despite its many methodological faults and lack of consistent replication (see Harris, 1979; Winter, 1996, pp. 531-534). At Yale Uni­versity during the 1930s and 1940s, the Institute of Human Relations under the leadership of Hull tried to bring about a synthesis of psycho­analytic theory and an experimental psychology self-consciously striving for immaculate purity of method (see Morawski, 1986). One result was a series of experimental stuthes of psychoanalytic concepts (Sears, 1943); another was the Miller-Dollard learning-based theory of personality (Dollard & Miller, 1950; Miller & Dollard, 1941). At the behaviorist extreme, Skinner (1953) proposed to dispense with personality theory and constructs altogether, in favor of the concept of reinforcement history.
The Rise and Fall of Culture and Personality
The culture and personality movement was born in the 1930s, as anthropologists and psychoana­lysts became interested in each other's disciplines (see Inkeles & Levinson, 1954; Kluckhohn, 1954). The underlying theoretical notion was that a cultures distinctive pattern of childrear-ing, derived from its broader characteristics and values, formed distinctive personalities in its children; thus the adults in each culture would


have similar or "modal" adult personalities (see Benedict, 1946, for a culture and personality study of Japan). Early financial support came from such sources as the Social Sciences Re­search Council's Committee on Personality and Culture to support cross-cultural stuthes (c.g., Mead, 1937). Later, during World War II, cul­ture and personality research contributed to the Allied war effort with stuthes of enemies (and al­lies), as well as methodological handbooks (see examples cited in Inkeles & Levinson, 1954).
After the war, culture and personality stuthes of particular cultures were collected in inter­disciplinary readers (Kluckhohn, Murray, & Schneider, 1953; see also Y. Cohen, 1961; and LeVine, 1982). Interdisciplinary programs, such as Harvard's Department of Social Relations evolved to support the enterprise. The develop­ment of the Human Relations Area Files (Mur-dock, 1982) made possible cross-cultural research on personality (e.g., Whiting & Child, 1953). By the late 1950s, however, the culture and per­sonality movement seemed to have run its course. With the hot war won and the Cold War dominated by political scientists and economists operating in an atmosphere of suspicion and orthodoxy, the intellectual climate had dear­ly changed. Moreover, the methodological and conceptual critiques leveled by Inkeles and Levinson (1954) met widi no real response. Cul­ture and personality research, they argued, over­simplified "culture" by assuming uniformity and homogeneity, while neglecting social structure. The field relied almost exclusively on impres­sionistic or small-sample methods. By the early 1960s, culture and personality had almost disap­peared, although some researchers continued to develop new methods and carry out cross-national personality research (e.g., Barren & Eysenck, 1984; McClelland, 1961). House (1981) offered a sophisticated delineation of the distinctive influences of social structure and cul­ture in relation to personality.
MischePs Critique and Personality's Response
Although its title, Personality and Assessment, was innocent enough, Mischel's book (1968) had the effect of a bombshell. Reviewing the field, Mischel claimed that the usefulness of broad dis-positional personality variables had been seri­ously overstated, because (1) such variables did not show cross-situational or temporal consis­tency, and (2) they were not highly correlated

with behavioral outcomes."The term "personal­ity coefficient" was derisively applied to any cor­relation "between .20 and .30 which is found persistently when virtually any personality di­mension inferred from a questionnaire is related to almost any conceivable external criterion.... Generally such correlations are too low to have value for most individual assessment purposes" (Mischel, 1968, pp. 77-78). In place of the usual array of personality variables, Mischel advocated the use of a highly specific, almost idiographic, version of social learning theory. Later Mischel and Shoda (1995; see also Mischel and Shoda, Chapter 7, this volume) emphasized cognitive variables, so that his theory, along with that of Bandura (1989; see also Chapter 6, this volume), became known as social cognitive theory.
After some initial confusion and disorienta-tion, personality psychologists found their bear­ings and replied (sec Winter, 1996, Chap. 16). Block (1977) criticized Mischel s review of the literature as selective and partial, weighted heav­ily with stuthes involving one-shot experimental responses (Cattell's T data). Epstein (1979) showed that aggregating one-shot dependent variables across situations produced stronger re­lationships to personality. Funder and Ozer (1983) demonstrated that classic "situarionaT findings from social psychology, when expressed as correlations, were of the same magnitudes as "personality coefficients." Stuthes showed indi­vidual differences in consistency and in the ex­planatory power of situational versus personality variables (see Winter, 1996, pp. 582-584), and demonstrated interaction between personality and situation—either cross-sectional (Magnus-son & Endler, 1977) or temporal (Caspi, Elder, & Bern, 1987). Researchers measured the precise nature of situations (Bern & Funder, 1978) and used context variables as moderators in relating personality to behavior and outcome. Moreover, as D. Buss (1987) pointed out, personality can affect situations, through processes of selection, evocation, and manipulation.
Cultural and Cross-Cultural Psychology
During the last decade of the century, perhaps as a response to the increasing globalization of eco­nomic, social, and intellectual life, personality psychology began to be influenced by the per­spectives of cultural psychology (see Cole, 1996; Cross 6c Markus, Chapter 15, this volume; Shweder, 1991; Shweder & Sullivan, 1990). Some researchers stuthed psychological processes


in particular cultures; others sought a more com­prehensive cross-cultural framework (see Berry, Dasen, & Saraswathi, 1997). Hofstede (1980) identified four dimensions along which cultures can be compared: individualism-collectivism, power distance, orientation to uncertainty, and gendering of male-female relations. So far, though, only the first dimension has received much attention from personality psychologists interested in culture, as for example in the dis­cussion of "self" by Markus and Kitayama (1991).
FUTURE DIRECTIONS AND THE "LESSONS OF HISTORY" FOR PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY
What lessons have we learned from our survey of the last century of personality psychology? What are the likely directions of future advance? Pre­dictions are always perilous, but we permit our­selves two observations about likely future trends during the next few decades.
Importance of Context
First, we believe that personality psychology will need to pay increased attention to matters of context. Whatever the evolutionary origins, ge­netic basis, or physiological substrate of any as­pect of personality, both its level and channels of expression will be strongly affected, in complex ways, by the multiple dimensions of social con­text: not only the immediate situational context, but also the larger contexts of age cohort, family, institution, social class, nation/culture, history, and (perhaps supremely) gender. We suggest that varying the social macrocontext will "constel­late," or completely change, all other variables of personality—much as in the classic demonstra­tions of gestalt principles of perception.
To give a simple illustration (see Winter, 1996, p. 586): Personality variables, such as sur-gency, conscientiousness, power motivation, or attributional style may (or may not) have evolu­tionary, genetic, or physiological aspects. But consider how differently these variables would have been expressed on the morning of June 6, 1944, by (1) a white 20-year-old American man storming "Utah" Beach during the World War II Normandy invasion, and (2) a middle-aged Japanese American woman held in an "intern­ment camp" for U.S. citizens and residents of Japanese ancestry, in the Utah (U.S.) desert.

 


Most macrocontexts involve power differen­tials or hegemony. As a kind of "force-field" (see Lewin, 1935), social power therefore affects the formation and expression of personality. This would be obvious in many other social sciences and humanities, influenced as they have been by Foucault, deconstruccionism, feminist theory, and critical theory. Although psychology has been largely isolated from these intellectual cur­rents, we believe that the field of personality can continue to ignore them only at its peril.
• Complexity of Perspective
Personality is inherendy complex, the result of many interacting forces ranging from biology to history. The postmodernist perspective merely emphasizes this complexity. To consider just one personality variable: Just as there is not a single "text" in literature, so also there may not be a single "self but rather many selves. We suggest the following analogy: Personality may come to be seen as a series of interacting elements, all ex­isting like a series of Windows computer appli­cations. Over time, different personality "appli­cations" are installed, opened, moved between foreground and background, modified, closed, even deleted. Although the sum total of available "personality" elements may have limits that are specifiable (though perhaps unique for each per­son), the current "on-line" personality may be complex and fluid.
In other fields of science, recognition of in­creased complexity has led to the development of "chaos theory" or "complexity theory," which is now being taken up by psychologists (e.g., Vallacher & Nowak, 1997). Because two ba­sic postulates of personality psychology are (1) complexity of interaction among elements and (2) that earlier experience affects later be­havior in ways that are at least somewhat irre­versible (or reversible with greater difficulty than acquisition), the field seems ideally situated to take advantage of these new theoretical and methodological tools.
Diversity of People, Diversity of Personality Psychologists
A final "lesson" from the history of personality involves the background and training of many of its great figures over the past century. Consider that Freud started as a neurologist; Murray, as a surgeon with a doctorate in biochemistry; and Cattelk, with an extensive background in statis-


tics. Rogers enrolled in theological seminary be­fore he turned to psychology, Eysenck originally planned to study physics, and Kelly was first an aeronautical engineer and then taught public speaking. Even Gordon Allport blended his stuthes of psychology with social ethics.
As a field, therefore, personality has been enormously enriched by the experiences and perspectives that these people brought from their original fields and interests. As our society increasingly recognizes the diversity of the per­sons we claim to interpret and analyze, personal­ity psychology needs to insure that its theorists and researchers in the 21st century are drawn from a diverse (academic and extraacademic) background and that they are given diverse expe­riences as a pan of their professional training.
NOTES
1. This is only one possible construction; in many cultures (China, for example) it would be unusual, if not impossible, to conceive of a skin-bounded indi­vidual body without at the same time considering the family (or kin-group, tribe, neighborhood, and na­tion).
2. Gallon's work is one of the major foundations of correlation and psychological testing. It served his convictions that most human abilites were innate and that British civilization was inherently superior, and his advocacy of eugenics. These uncomfortable so­ciopolitical facts must be noted, because by shaping the ways in which questions are framed and the puta-tively "adequate" methods of answering them are evaluated, they may continue to exert subde effects long after being ovcitly discarded. Thus the concep­tions of "intelligence" implied by the methods of Gal-ton and others influenced (from today's perspective, seriously impeded) our understanding of the family of multiple "intelligences."
3. Allport later tried (without success) to replace these terms with "dimensional" and "morphogenic," respectively (1962, p. 407).
4. CattcU's "specification equation" is perhaps the most fully developed example (1957, pp. 302-306) of such prediction equations.
5. The history of research on the authoritarian personality is illustrative and instructive in this re­spect. Conceived originally as a critique of existing structures of social and economic power, it became transformed into a measure of individual pathology as it moved "from Berlin to Berkeley" (Samelson, 1993).
6. Even Murray's biographical study of Melville remained largely unpublished (Robinson, 1992).


7. One extraordinary recent example is Nasby and Read's (1997) 283-page case study of Dodge Morgan, integrating analytic and interpretive ap­proaches, published as an entire issue of the Journal of Personality.
8.  See, for example, M. Smith (1990, p. 539).
9. A review of several stuthes of "character and temperament" had appeared 5 years earlier (Thur-stone, 1916).
10. In 1925, when it was taken over by APA, it was renamed Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy­chology.
11. Although Allport's dissertation appears to have been the first on personality traits, a dissertation on character traits had appeared the previous year (Filter, 1921).
12. Like Gaiton and other early measurement theorists, Cartcll endorsed the principles of eugenics (1987). His views on this topic—for example, that from an evolutionary perspective, democracy might be an "injustice and an unethical procedure" because it fosters the "parasitism" of the "less gifted" upon the "brains" of the "intelligent management and in­ventor types" (p. 113)—aroused considerable contro­versy when they became widely known in the late 1990s.
13. To this assertion, however, Cattell added an amusing postscript: "If machines had tongues they might rate human beings according to certain ma­chine-handling traits ... not... observable by or im­portant to other human beings, while among dogs there might well be terms much concerned with the way human beings use their feet" (1946, p. 216).
14. Murray stuthed several additional needs less formally.
15. Some behaviorism simply ridiculed such con­cepts as the unconscious, whereas others tried to translate them; for example, as "behavior that has be­come unverbalizable, due to conflicting reinforce­ment contingencies."
16. Loevinger's concept and measure of ego devel­opment (1976), although directly drawn from psychoanalysis, shares substantial construct and em­pirical validity with cognitive complexity measures.
17. Later (e.g., Allport, 1961) the prof rium or "es­sential selfhood."
18. Perhaps this support reflected a realization, on the part of significant elite groups, of growing U.S. world power and influence. In an analogous way, the waxing of the British Empire in the 19th century was associated with support for the emergence of anthro­pology as a discipline.
19. Mischcl was not alone in these criticisms: Peterson (1965) and Vcrnon (1964) raised similar points at about the same time.


REFERENCES
Allport. F. H., & Allport, G. W. (1921). Personality traits: Their classification and measurement. Journal of Abnormal Psychology and Social Psychology, 16, 6-40.
Allport, G. W. (1921). Personality and character. Psycho­logical Bulletin, 18,441-455.
Allport, G. W. (1923). The Leipzig Congress of Psychol­ogy. American Journal of Psychology, 34, 612-615.
Allport, G. W. (1924a). The standpoint of Gestalt psy­chology. Psyche, 4, 354-361.
Allport, G. W. (1924b). The study of the undivided per­sonality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 19, 132-141.
Allport, G. W. (1927). Concepts of trait and personality. Psychological Bulletin, 24, 284-293.
Allport, G. W (1928). A test for ascendance-submis­sion. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 23, 118-136.
Allport, G. W. (1929). The study of personality by the intuitive method: An experiment in teaching from The Locomotive God. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 24, 14-27.
Allport, G. W. (1931). What is a trait of personality? Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 25, 368-372.
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological inter' pretation. New York: Henry Holt.
Allport, G. W. (1942). The use of personal documents in psychological science. New York: Social Science Re­search Council.
Allport, G. W (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Allport, G, W. (1962). The general and the unique in psychological science. Journal of Personality, 30, 405-422.
Allport, G. W. (1965). Letters from Jenny. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.
Allport, G. W. (1967). Gordon W. Allport. In E. G. Bor­ing & G. Lindzey (Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 5, pp. 1-25). New York: Apple-ton-Century-Crofts.
Allport, G. W, & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait-names: A psycho-lexical study. Psychological" Monographs, 47(1, Whole No. 211), 1-171.
Allport, G. W, & Vemon, P. E. (1930). The field of per­sonality. Psychological Bulletin, 27, 677-730.
Atkinson, J. W. (1982). Motivational determinants of thematic apperception. In A. J. Stewart (Ed.), Moti­vation and society (pp. 3—40). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Chi­cago: Rand McNally.
Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44, 1175-1184.
Barcnbaum, N. B. (1997a). The case(s) of Gordon Allport. Journal of Personality, 65, 743-755.
Barcnbaum, N. B. (1997b, April). The general and the unique: William Stern's "considerable influence" on Gordon Allport. In J. T. Lamiell (Chair), William

 


Stern's critical penonalism in contemporary psychology. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the East­ern Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
Barenbaum, N. B. (1997c. June). "The most revealing method of all": Gordon AUport and case stuthes. Paper presented at the annual meeting of Cheiron, Rich­mond, VA..
Bargh, J. A. (1982). Attention and automaticiry in the processing of self-relevant information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 43, 425-436.
Barren, P.. & Eysenck, S. B. (1984). The assessment of personality factors across 25 countries. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 615-632.
Bellih, R. N.. Madsen, R., Sullivan, W. M-, Swidler, A., & Tipton, S. M. (1985). Habits of the heart: Indi­vidualism and commitment in American life. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.
Bern, D. J., & Funder, D. C. (1978). Predicting more of the people more of the time: Assessing the personal­ity of situations. Psychological Review, 85, 485-501.
Benedict, R. (1946). The chrysanthemum and the sword: Patterns of Japanese culture. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Berry, J. W, Dasen, P. R., Saraswathi, T. S. (Eds.). (1997). Handbook of cross-cultural psychology: Vol. 2. Basic processes and human development. Boston: Al-lyn & Bacon.
Bills, A. G. (1938). Changing views of psychology as sci­ence. Psychological Review, 45, 377-394.
Blake, R. R., & Ramscy, G. V. (Eds.). (1951). Perception: An approach to personality. New York: Ronald Press.
Block, J. (1971). Lives through time. Berkeley, CA: Ban­croft Books.
Block, J. (1977). Advancing the psychology of personal­ity: Paradigmatic shift or improving the quality of research? In D. Magnusson & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Personality at the crossroads: Current issues in interac­tional psychology (pp. 37-63). Hillsdale, NJ: Ertbaum.
Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor ap­proach to personality. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187-215.
Bray, D. W. (1982). The assessment center and the study of lives. American Psychologist, 37, 180-189.
Burckhardt, J. (1954). The civilization of the Renaissance in Italy. New York: Modern Library. (Original work published 1860)
Burnham, J. C. (1968). Historical background for the study of personality. In E. F. Borgatta &C W. W. Lambert (Eds.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 3-81). Chicago: Rand McNally.
Buss, A. H. (1989). Personality as traits. American Psy­chologist. 44, 1378-1388.
Buss, D. M. (1987). Selection, evocation, and manipula­tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1214-1221.
Buss, D. M. (Ed.). (1990). Biological foundations of personality. Evolution, behavioral genetics, and psy-chophysiology [Special issue]. Journal of 'Personality, 55(1).


Camfield, T. M. (1969). Psychologists at war: The history of American psychology and the First World War. Un­published doctoral dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.
Campbell, D. T, & Fiskc. D. W. (1959). Conver­gent and discriminant validity by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81-105.
Cantor, N. (1990). From thought to behavior: "Having" and "doing" in the study of personality and cogni­tion. American Psychologist, 45, 735-750.
Cantor, N., & Zirkcl, S. (1990). Personality, cognition, and purposive behavior. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Hand­book of personality: Theory and research (pp. 135-164). New York: Guilford Press.
Carbon, R. (1971). Where is the person in personality research? Psychological Bulletin, 75, 203-219.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control theory approach to human be­havior. New York: Springcr-Verlag.
Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Bern, D. J. (1987). Mov­ing against the world: Life-course patterns of explo­sive children. Developmental Psychology, 23, 308-313.
Cattell, R. B. (1945). The description of personality. Ill: Principles and findings in a factor analysis. American Journal of Psychology, 58, 69-90.
Cattell, R. B. (1946). Description and measurement of personality. Yonkers-on-Hudson, NY: World Book.
Cattell, R. B. (1957). Personality and motivation structure and measurement. Yonkers-on-Hudson, NY: World Book.
Cattell, R. B. (1987). Beyondism: Religion from science. New York: Praeger.
Cloninger, C. R. (1991). Brain networks underlying per­sonality development. In B. J. Carroll & J. E. Bar-rett (Eds.), Psychopathology and the brain (pp. 183-208). New York: Raven Press.
Cloninger, C. R. (1998). The genetics and psychobiol-ogy of the seven-factor model of personality. In K. R. Silk (Ed.), Biology of personality disorders (pp. 63-92). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Cohen, S. (1983). The mental hygiene movement, the development of personality and the school: The medicalization of American education. History of Education Quarterly, 23, 123-149.
Cohen, Y. A. (1961). Social structure and personality: A casebook. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Cohler, B. J. (1993). Describing lives: Gordon Allport and the "science" of personality. In K. H. Craik, R. Hogan, & R. N. Wolfe (Eds.), Fijty years of personal­ity psychology (pp. 131-146). New York: Plenum Press.
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Costa, E T, & McCrae, R. R. (1992a). Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and Individual Differ­ences, 13. 653-665.


Costa, n T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992b). Tour ways five /actors are not basic": Reply. Personality and Individ­ual Differences, 13, 861-865.
Craik, K. H., Hogan, R., & Wolfe, R. N. (Eds.). (1993). Fifty yean of personality psychology. New York: Ple­num Press.
Cramer, P (1991). The development of defense mecha­nisms; Theory, research, and assessment. New York: Springer-Vcrlag.
Cronbach, L J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the inter­nal structure of tests. Ptychometrika, 16, 297-334.
Danziger, K. (1990). Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psychological research. New York. Cam­bridge University Press.
Danziger, K. (1997). Naming the mind; How psychology found its language. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York Ple­num Press.
Dickson, D. H., & Kelly, I. W. (1985). The "Barnum ef­fect* in personality assessment: A review of the lit­erature. Psychological Reports, 57, 367-382.
Dollard, J., & Miller. N. E. (1950). Personality and psy­chotherapy; An analysis in terms of learning, thinking, and culture. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Elliott, R. M. (1939). The Harvard Explorations in per­sonality [Review of the book Explorations in personal­ity: A clinical and experimental study of fifty men of college age]. American Journal of Psychology, 52, 453-462.
Emmons, R. A., & McAdams, D. R (1995). Levels and domains in personality [Special issue]. Journal of Personality, 63(3).
Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior I. On pre­dicting most of the people much of the time. Jour­nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1097-1126
Erdelyi, M. H. (1974). A new look at the New Look: Perceptual defense and vigilance. Psychological Re­view 81, 1-25.
Erdelyi, M. H. (1985). Psychoanalysis; Freud's cognitive psychology. New York: Freeman.
Eriksen, C. W., & Pierce, J. (1968). Defense mecha­nisms. In E. F. Borgatta & W W Lambert (Eds.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 1007-1040). Chicago: Rand McNally.
Erikson, E H. (1958). Young man Luther. New York: Norton.
Erikson, E H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.). New Yorlc Norton. (Original work published 1950)
Erikson, E. H. (1969). Gandhis truth. New York: Norton.
Erikson, E. H. (1980). Identity and the lift cycle. New York: Norton. (Original work published 1959)
Eysenck, H. J. (1957). The dynamics of anxiety and hys­teria. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas, Publisher.
Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of person­ality. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 244-276). New York: Guil-ford Press.


Eysenck, H. J. (1992a). Four ways five factors arc not ba­sic. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 667-673.
Eysenck, H. J. (1992b). A reply to Costa and McCrae: P or A and C—the role of theory. Personality and Indi­vidual Differences, 13(8), 867-868.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and individual differences: A natural science approach. New York: Plenum Press.
Fernald, G. G. (1920). Character vs. intelligence in per­sonality stuthes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 15, 1-10.
Filter, R. O. (1921). An experimental study of character mats. Journal of Applied Psychology. 5, 297-317.
Fisher, S., & Greenberg, R. R (1977). The scientific credi­bility of Freud's theories and therapy. New Yorlc Basic Books.
Fleming, J. (1982). Pro/ectivc and psychometric ap­proaches to measurement: The case of fear of suc­cess. In A. J. Stewart (Ed.), Motivation and society (pp. 63-96). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Franz, C. E, & Stewart, A. J. (Eds.). (1994). Women cre­ating lives: Identities, resilience, and resistance. Boul­der, CO: Westvicw Press.
Freud, A. (1946). The ego and the mechanisms of defense. New Yorlc International Universities Press. (Origi­nal work published 1937)
Freud, S. (1955). Beyond the pleasure principle. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works ofSigmund Freud (Vol. 18, pp. 7-64). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1920)
Freud, S. (1957). Some character types met with in psy­cho-analytic work. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The stand' ard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Fnud (Vol. 14, pp. 309-333). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1916)
Freud, S. (1959a). Character and anal erotism. In J. Stra­chey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psy­chological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 9, pp. 169-175). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1908)
Freud, S. (1959b). On the sexual theories of children. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of thf complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 9, pp. 209-226). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1908)
Freud, S. (1961-1963). Introductory lectures on psy­choanalysis. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edi­tion of the complete psychological works ofSigmund Freud (Vols. 15-16). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1916-1917)
Freud, S. (1964). New introductory lectures on psycho­analysis. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 22, pp. 5-182). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1933)
Fundcr, D. C., & Ozer, D. J. (1983). Behavior as a func­tion of the situation. Journal of 'Personality and'Social Psychology, 44, 107-112.

 


Galton, F. (1884). Measurement of character. Fortnightly Review, 36, 179-185.
Garrary, J. A. (1981). Gordon Allport's rules for the preparation of life histories and case stuthes. Biogra­phy, 4, 283-292.
Gergen, K. J. (1991). The saturated self. Dilemmas of identity in contemporary life. New York: Bask Books.
Gergen, K. J., & Gordon, C. (Eds.). (1968). The self in social interaction. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Win-stem.
Gieser, L.. & Stein, M. I. (Eds.), (in press). Evocative im­ages: The Thematic Apperception Test and the art of projection. Washington, DC: APA Boob.
Goldberg, L R. (1992). The development of markers for * the Big'Rve factor structure. Psychological Assess­ment, 4. 26-42.
Gough, H. G. (1957). California Psychological Inventory: Manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Gough, H. G. (1987). California Psychological Inventory: Administrator's guide. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck's theory of per­sonality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for person­ality (pp. 246-276). New York Springer.
Gray, J. A. (1987). Perspectives on anxiety and impulsiv-ity: A. commentary. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 493-509.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit so­cial cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereo­types. Psychological Review, 102, 4-27.
GuilfonLJ. R (1938). [Review of the book Personality: A psychological interpretation]. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 33, 414-420.
Gurin, P.. Miller, A. H., & Gurin, G. (1980). Stratum identification and consciousness. Social Psychology Quarterly, 43, 30-47.
Hale, N. G. (1971). Freud and the Americans: The begin­nings of psychoanalysis in the United States, 1876-1917. New York: Oxford University Press.
Harris, B. (1979). Whatever happened to Little Albert? American Psychologist, 34, 151-160.
Helgeson, V S. (1994). Relation of agency and com­munion to well-being: Evidence and potential ex­planations. Psychological Bulletin. 116(3), 412-428.
Hermans, H. J. M., Kempen, H. J. G., & van Loon, R. J. R (1992). The dialogical self: Beyond individual­ism and rationalism. American Psychologist, 47, 23-33.
Hofstede, G. H. (1980). Culture's consequences: Interna­tional differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hollingworch, H. L. (1938). [Review of the book Per­sonality: A psychological interpretation}. Psychological Bulletin. 35, 103-107.
House, J. S. (1981). Social structure and personality. In M. Rosenbcrg & R. H. Turner (Eds.), Social psychol­ogy: Sociological perspectives (pp. 525-561). New York: Basic Books.
Inkelcs, A., &c Levinson, D. J. (1954). National charac­ter: The study of modal personality and sociocultu-


ral systems. In G. Lindzcy (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 977-1020). Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Jackson, D. N. (1974). The Personality Research Form. Port Huron, MI: Research Psychologists Press.
Jankowicz, A. D. (1987). Whatever became of George Kelly? Applications and implications. American Psy­chologist, 42(5), 481-487.
Jcnkins, J. G. (1938). [Review of the book Personality: A psychological interpretation]. American Journal of Psy­chology, 51, 777-778.
John, O. P (1990). The "Big Five" ractor taxonomy: Di­mensions of personality in the natural language and in questionnaires. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 66-100). New York: Guilfbrd Press.
John, O. E, & Robins, R. W. (1993). Gordon Allport: Father and critic of the five-factor model. In K. H. Craik, R. Hogan, & R. N. Wolfe (Eds.), Fifyyears of personality psychology (pp. 215-236). New York: Plenum Press.
John, O. E, & Robins, R. W (1994). Accuracy and bias in self-perception: Individual differences in self-enhancement and the role of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 206-219.
Jung, C. G. (1971). Psychological types. In H. Read, M. Fundham, G. Adler, & W. McGuire (Eds.), The col­lected works of C. G. Jung (Vol. 6, pp. 1-495). Princeton: Princeton University Press. (Original work published 1923)
Jung, C. G., von Franz, M.-L., Henderson, J. L., Jacobi, J., & JafFe, A. (1964). Man and his symbols. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. 2 vols. New York Norton.
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1990). The psychological unconscious. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 445-464). New York Guilford Press.
Kluckhohn,   C. (1954). Culture and behavior. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 921-976). Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Kluckhohn, C., Murray, H. A., & Schneidcr, D. M. (1953). Personality in nature, society, and culture. New York Knopf.
Kohut, H. (1985). Self psychology and the humanities. New York Norton.
Lamicll, J. T. (1997). Individuals and the differences be­tween them. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 117-141). San Thego: Academic Press.
Lamiell, J. T. (1998). "Nomothetic" and "idiographic": Contrasting Windclband's understanding with con­temporary usage. Theory and Psychology, 8, 23-38.
Lasch, C. L (1979). The culture of narcissism. New York: Norton.
Laughiin, C. D. (1973). Discussion: The influence of Whitehead's organism upon Murray's pcrsonology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 9, 251-257.


LeVine, R. A. (1982). Culture, behavior and personality (2nd ed.). Chicago: Aldine.
Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Loevinger, J. (1976). Ego development: Conceptions and theories. San Francisco: Josscy-Bass.
MacKinnon, D. W. (1975). IPAR's contribution to the conceptualization and study of creativity. In I. A. Taylor & J. W. Getzels (Eds.), Perspectives in creativ­ity (pp. 60-89). Chicago: Aldine.
Magnusson, D., & Endler, N. S. (1977). Personality at the crossroads: Current issues in interactional psychol­ogy. Hiilsdale, NJ: Erlbaurn.
Marcta, J. E., Waterman, A. S., Mattcson, D. R., Archer, S. L, & Orlofsky, J. L. (1993). Ego identity: A hand­book for psychosocial research. New York: Springcr-Verlag.
Markus, H., & Cross, S. (1990). The interpersonal self. In L Pcrvin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 576-608). New York: Guilford Press.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and moti­vation. Psychological Review 98, 224-253.
McAdaim, D. R (1985). Power, intimacy, and the lift story. Homewood, IL: Dorsey.
McAdarm, D. E (1997). A conceptual history of person­ality psychology. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 3-39). San Thego: Academic Press.
McAdams, D. R, & Ochberg, R. L. (Eds.). (1988). Psy-chobiography and life narratives [Special issue]. Journal of Personality, 56"(1)-
McAdams, D. E, & West, S. G. (1997). Introduction: Personality psychology and the case study. Journal of Personality. 65, 757-783.
McClelland, D. C. (1961). The achieving society. Prince-ton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
McClelland, D. C. (1989). Motivational factors in health and disease. American Psychologist, 44, 675-683.
McClelland, D. C, Atkinson. J. W., dark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L (1953). The achievement motive. New York: Applcron-Century-Crofts.
McLeod, J. (1992). The story of Henry Murray's diag­nostic council: A case study in the demise of a scien­tific method. Clinical Psychology Forum, 44, 6-12.
Mead, M. (Ed.). (1937). Cooperation and competition among primitive peoples. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. (1941). Social learning and imitation. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley.
Mischel. W.. & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality. Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246-268.
Morawski, J. (1986). Organizing knowledge and behav­ior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations, bis, 77, 219-242.


Morgan, C D., & Murray, H. A. (1935). A method for investigating fantasies: The Thematic Apperception Test. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 34, 289-306.
Murdock, G. P (Ed.). (1982). Outline of cultural materi­als (5th ed.). New Haven, CT: Human Relations Area Hies.
Murphy, G. (1932). An historical introduction to modern psychology (4th rev. ed.). New York: Harcourt, Brace.
Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford University Press.
Murray, H. A. (1940). What should psychologists do about psychoanalysis? Journal of Abnormal and So­cial Psychology, 35, 150-175.
Murray, H. A. (1951). Toward a classification of interac­tions. In T. Parsons & E. A. Shils (Eds.), Toward a general theory of action (pp. 434—464). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Murray, H. A. (1959). Preparations for the scaffold of a comprehensive system. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: Study of a science (Vol. 3, pp. 7-54). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Murray, H. A. (1967). Henry A. Murray. In E. G. Boring & G. Lindzey (Eds.), A history of psychology in auto­biography (Vol. 5, pp. 283-310). New York: Apple-ton-Century-Crofts.
Murray, H. A. (1968). Components of an evolving per-sonological system. In D. L. Sills (Ed.), Interna­tional encyclopedia of the social sciences (Vol. 12, pp. 5-13). New York: Macmillan-Free Press.
Murray, H. A. (1977). Indispensables for the making, testing, and remaking of a personological system. In R. W. Rieber & K. Salzinger (Eds.), The roots of American psychology: Historical influences and impli­cations for the future (pp. 323-331). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Murray, H. A. (1981). An American Icarus. In: E. S. Shneidman (Ed.), Endeavors in psychology: Selections from the personology of Henry A. Murray (pp. 535-556). New York: Harper & Row. (Original work published 1955)
Myers,  I. (1962). The Myen-Briggs Type Indicator. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. •
Nasby, W., & Read, N. W. (1997). The life voyage of a solo circumnavigator. Integrating theoretical and methodological perspectives. Journal of Personality, 65, 785-1068.
Nicholson, I. A. M. (1997a). Humanistic psychology and intellectual identity: The "open" system of Gor­don Allport. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 37(3), 61-79.
Nicholson, I. A. M. (I997b). To "correlate psychology and social ethics": Gordon Allport and the first course in American personality psychology. Journal of Personality, 65. 733-742.
Nicholson, I. A. M. (1998). Gordon Allport, character, and the "culture of personality," 1897-1937. His­tory of Psychology, 1, 52-<S8.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental proc­esses. Psychological Review, 84, 231-259.


INTRODUCTION


Only spectacular crimes grab headlines in L.A. (1994, February 19). Toronto Globe and Mail, p. A12.
Pandora, K. (1997). Rebels within the ranks: Psychologists critique of scientific authority and democratic realities in New Deal America. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni­versity Press.
Parker, J. D. A. (1991). In search of the person: The his­torical development of American personality psychol' ogy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, York University, Toronto.
Peale, N. V. (1952). The poiver of positive thinking. New York: Prentice-Hall.
Pervin, L. A. (1983). The stasis and flow of behavior To­ward a theory of goals. In M. M. Page (Ed.), Ne­braska Symposium on Motivation 1982 (pp. 1-53). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Pervin, L. A. (Ed.). (1989). Goal concepts in personality and social psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaurn.
Pervin, L. A. (1990). A brief history of modern personal-icy theory. In L Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personal­ity: Theory and research (pp. 3~18). New York: Guilford Press.
Pervin, L. A. (1994). A critical analysis of current trait theory. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 103-113.
Peterson, D. R. (1965). Scope and generality of verbally defined personality factors. Psychological Review, 72, 48-59.
Poffenberger, A. T. (1922). Measures of intelligence and chancier. Journal of Philosophy, 19, 261-266.
Polansky, N. A. (1941). How shall a life-history be writ­ten? Character and Personality, 9, 188-207.
Pressey, S. L. (1921). A group scale for investigating the emotions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology and Social Psychology, 16, 55-64.
Prince, M., & Allport, F H. (1921). Editorial an­nouncement. Journal of Abnormal Psychology and Social Psychology, 16, 1-5.
Prince, M., & Moore, H. T. (1928). Editorial: The cur­rent number. Journal of Abnormal Psychology and So­cial Psychology. 23. 117.
Robinson, F. G. (1992). Love's story told: A life of Henry A. Murray. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Rogers, C. R. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality, and interpersonal relationships, as developed in the client-centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psy­chology: A study of a science (Vol. 3, pp. 184-256). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Rorer, L G. (1990). Personality assessment: A concep­tual survey. In L A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of per­sonality: Theory and research (pp. 693-720). New York: Guilford Press.
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books.
Rosenzweig, S. (1943). The ghost of Henry James: A study in thematic apperception. Character & Person­ality, 12. 79-100.
Run/an, W. M. (1997). Studying lives: Psychobiography and the conceptual structure of personality psychol­ogy. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.),


Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 41-69). San Thego: Academic Press.
Samelson, F. (1993). The authoritarian character from Berlin to Berkeley and beyond: The odyssey of a problem. In W. F. Stone, G. Lcderer, & R. Christie (Eds.), Strength and weakness: The authoritarian per­sonality today (pp. 22-43). New York: Springer.
Sampson, E. E. (1981). Cognitive psychology as ideol­ogy. American Psychologist, 36, 730-743.
Sanford, R. N. (1963). Personality: Its place in psychol­ogy. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 5, pp. 488-592). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Santa/ana, G. (1906). The life of reason: Or the phases of human progress. I. Introduction and reason in com' man sense. New York: Scribncrs.
Schaffer, R, (1991). America in the Great War: The rise of the war welfare state. New York : Oxford University Press.
Sears, R. R. (1943). Survey of objective stuthes of psycho­analytic concepts: A report prepared for the Committee on Social Adjustment. New York: Social Science Re­search Council.
Shwedcr, R. A. (1991). Thinking through cultures: Expe­ditions in cultural psychology. Cambridge, MA: Har­vard University Press.
Shweder, R. A., & Sullivan, M. A. (1990). The semiotic subject of cultural psychology. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 399-416). New York: Guilford Press.
Singer, J. A, (1995). Seeing one's selfi Locating narrative memory in a framework of personality. Journal of Personality, 63, 429-457.
Skaggs, E. B. (1945). Personalistic psychology as science. Psychological Review. 52, 234-238.
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.
Smith, C. P. (Ed.). (1992). Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis. New York; Cambridge University Press.
Smith, M. B. (1990). Personology launched [Review of the book Explorations in personality: A clinical and experimental study of fifty men of college age]. Con­temporary Psychology, 35, 537-539.
Snyder, M. (1987). Public appearances, private realities: The psychology of self-monitoring. New York: Free­man.
Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement: Two meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 140-154.
Stagncr, R. (1937). Psychology of personality. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Susman, W. I. (1979). "Personality" and the making of twentieth-century culture. In J. Higham & P. K. Conklin (Eds.), New directions in American intellec­tual history (pp. 212-226). Baltimore: Johns Hop-kins University Press.
Tajfel, H. (Ed.). (1982). Social identity and intergroup re­lations. London: Cambridge University Press.
Thomdike, E. L. (1914). Units and scales for measuring educational products. Proceedings of a Conference on


Educational Measurements. Indiana University Bulle­tin. 72(10), 128-141.
Thorne, A., & Gough, H. G. (1991). Portraits of type: An MBTI research compendium. Palo Alto, CA: Con­sulting Psychologists Press.
Thurstonc, L L. (1916). Character and temperament. Psychological Bulletin, 13, 384-388.
Triplet, R. G. (1983). Henry A. Murray ana1 the Harvard Psychological Clinic, 1926-1938: A struggle to ex­pand the disciplinary boundaries of academic psychol­ogy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of New Hampshire, Durham.
Triplet, R. G. (1992). Henry A. Murray: The making of a psychologist? American Psychologist, 47(2), 299-307.
Vallacher, R. R., & Nowak, A. (1997). The emergence of dynamical social psychology. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 73-99.
Vernon, P. E. (1933). The American v. the German methods of approach to the study of temperament and personality. British Journal of Psychology 24, 156-177.
Vernon, P. E. (1964). Personality assessment: A critical sur-vey. New York: Wiley.
Vernon, P E., & Allport, G. W. (1931). A test for per­sonal values. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychol­ogy, 26. 231-248.
Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emo­tional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 1-14.
Weinberger, J., & McClelland. D. C. (1990). Cognitive versus traditional motivational models: Irreconcil­able or complementary? In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cog­nition (Vol. 2, pp. 562-597). New York: Guilford Press.
Weinberger, J., & Silverman, L. H. (1990). Testability and empirical verification of psychoanalytic dy­namic propositions through subliminal psychody-namic activation. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 7, 299-339.
Weiner, B. (1990). Attribution in personality psychol­ogy. In L Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 465-485). New York: Guil­ford Press.
West, S. G. (1983). Personality and prediction: No-mothctic and idiographic approaches [Special is­sue}. Journal of Personality 57(3).
White. R. W. (1952). Lives in progress. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.


Whiting, J. W. M., & Child, I. L. (1953). Child training and personality: A cross-cultural study. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Wiggins, J. S. (1991). Agency and communion as con­ceptual coordinates for the understanding and measurement of interpersonal behavior. In D. Cic-chetti (Ed.), Thinking clearly about psychology: Essays in honor of Paul E. MeehL- VoL 2. Personality andpsy-chopathology (pp. 89-113). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Wiggins, J. S. (Ed.). (1996). The five-factor model of per­sonality: Theoretical perspectives. New York: Guilford Press.
Windelband, W. (1904). Gtschichte und Naturvnssen-schaft [History and natural science] (3rd ed.). Srrass-burg: Heitz. (Original work delivered 1894.)
Wink, R (1992). Three types of narcissism in women from college to mid-life. Journal of Personality, 60, 7-30.
Winter, D. G. (1993). Power, affiliation and war: Three tests of a motivational model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 532-545.
Winter, D. G. (1996). Personality: Analysis and interpre­tation of lives. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Winter, D. G. (1998). "Toward a science of personality psychology": David McClelland's development of empirically derived TAT measures. History of Psy­chology, 1, 130-153.
Winter, D. G., John, O. ?., Stcwart, A. J., Klohnen, E. C, & Duncan, L. E. (1998). Traits and motives: To­ward an integration of two traditions in personality research. Psychological Review, 105, 230-250.
Woodworth. R. S. (1919). Examination of emotional fitness for warfare. Psychological Bulletin, 15, 59-60.
Woodworth, R. S. (1932). Robert S. Woodworth. In C Murchison (Ed,), A history of psychology in autobiog­raphy (Vol. 2, pp. 359-380). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.
Wylie, R. C. (1974-1979). The self-concept. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
York, K. L, & John, O. R (1992). The four faces of Eve: A typological analysis of women's personality at midlife. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 494-508.

Young, K. (1928). The measurement of personal and so­cial traits. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 22, 431-442.
Zuckerman, M. (1991). Psychobiology of personality. New York: Cambridge University Press.


Source: http://www.rc.usf.edu/~jdorio/Personality/History%20of%20Modern%20Personality%20Theory%20and%20Research.doc

Web site to visit: http://www.rc.usf.edu/

Author of the text: indicated on the source document of the above text

If you are the author of the text above and you not agree to share your knowledge for teaching, research, scholarship (for fair use as indicated in the United States copyrigh low) please send us an e-mail and we will remove your text quickly. Fair use is a limitation and exception to the exclusive right granted by copyright law to the author of a creative work. In United States copyright law, fair use is a doctrine that permits limited use of copyrighted material without acquiring permission from the rights holders. Examples of fair use include commentary, search engines, criticism, news reporting, research, teaching, library archiving and scholarship. It provides for the legal, unlicensed citation or incorporation of copyrighted material in another author's work under a four-factor balancing test. (source: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Fair_use)

The information of medicine and health contained in the site are of a general nature and purpose which is purely informative and for this reason may not replace in any case, the council of a doctor or a qualified entity legally to the profession.

 

History of Modern Personality Theory and Research

 

The texts are the property of their respective authors and we thank them for giving us the opportunity to share for free to students, teachers and users of the Web their texts will used only for illustrative educational and scientific purposes only.

All the information in our site are given for nonprofit educational purposes

 

History of Modern Personality Theory and Research

 

 

Topics and Home
Contacts
Term of use, cookies e privacy

 

History of Modern Personality Theory and Research